Invented spelling intervention programmes: Comparing explicit and implicit instructions

Tiago Almeida, Ana Cristina Silva, João Rosa

Abstract


This study aims to compare the effectiveness of two invented spelling intervention programs, one with explicit instruction of graph-phonetics matches and another based on questioning and reflection on the graph-phonetic correspondences (implicit instructions). Ninety pre-school children, whose invented spellings use conventional letters unconventionally to represent sounds, were allocated to three groups, two experimental and one control. All groups were equivalent in age, intelligence, letter knowledge, and phonological awareness. We manipulated the type of instructions (implicit vs. explicit) between the pre- and post-tests in two experimental groups where children participated in an intervention programme of invented spelling. Children who participated in the implicit intervention programme showed a significant improvement in the number of correct letters mobilized in their spelling and phonemic awareness compared with children of control and explicit instruction group. Children from explicit instruction group showed significant more improvements than the children from the control group. These results suggest that questioning and reflection applied to invented spelling programmes seems to enhance a more significant knowledge about the relations between the oral and written code.


Keywords


Explicit instruction, Implicit instruction, Invented spelling, Interventions programmes, Preschool children.

Full Text:

PDF

References


Adams, M. (1998). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. MIT Press.

Albuquerque, A., & Alves Martins, M. (2017). Habilidades iniciais de alfabetização em português: Pesquisa transcultural em Portugal e no Brasil. Psico-USF, 22(3), 437-448. https://doi.org/10.1590/1413-82712017220305

Albuquerque, A., & Alves Martins, M. (2018). Escrita inventada no jardim-de-infância: Contributos para a aprendizagem da leitura e escrita. Análise Psicológica, 36, 341-354. https://doi.org/10.14417/ap.1308

Albuquerque, A., & Alves Martins, M. (2019). Enhancing children’s literacy learning: From invented spelling to effective reading and writing. L1 Educational Studies in Language and Literature, 19, 1-24. https://doi.org/10.17239/l1esll-2019.19.01.02.10.17239/l1esll-2019.19.01.02

Almeida, T., & Silva, C. (2019). Escritas inventadas no jardim-de-infância: Comparando dois programas de intervenção. Psicologia Escolar e Educacional, 23, 1-15. https://doi.org/10.1590/2175-35392019012948

Alvarado, M. (1998). Consciencia fonológica y escritura en ninos preescolares: La possibilidad de omitir el primer segmento. Lectura y Vida, 3, 42-50. http://fundacionzorrorojo.org/publicaciones/MoAl/19_03_Alvarado.pdf

Alves Martins, M. (1993). Évolution des conceptualisations d’un groupe d’enfants d’âge pré-scolaire sur l’écriture portugaise. Études de Linguistique Appliquée, 91, 60-69.

Alves Martins, M., Albuquerque, A., Almeida, T., Silva, A. C., Salvador, L., Rato, L., & Santos, A. (2020). Práticas de literacia em jardim-de-infância em Portugal: O que dizem os educadores. ISPA – Instituto Universitário.

Alves Martins, M., Albuquerque, A., Salvador, L., & Silva, C. (2013). The impact of invented spelling on early spelling and reading. Journal of Writing Research, 5, 215-237. https://doi.org/10.17239/jowr-2013.05.02.3

Alves Martins, M., Albuquerque, A., Salvador, L., & Silva, C. (2015). Escrita inventada e aquisição da leitura em crianças de idade pré-escolar. Psicologia: Teoria e Pesquisa, 31, 137-144. https://doi.org/10.1590/0102-37722015021639137144

Alves Martins, M., Salvador, L., Albuquerque, A., & Montanero Fernández, M. (2017). “Otro niño lo escribió así”. Ayuda educativa y resultados de actividades de escritura inventada. Revista de Educación, 377, 161-186. https://doi.org/10.4438/1988-592X-RE-2017-377-357

Alves Martins, M., Salvador, L., Albuquerque, A., & Silva, C. (2016). Invented spelling activities in small groups and early spelling and reading. Educational Psychology: An International Journal of Experimental Educational Psychology, 36, 738-752. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2014.950947

Alves Martins, M., & Silva, C. (2006a). The impact of invented spelling on phonemic awareness. Learning and Instruction, 16, 41-56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2005.12.005

Alves Martins, M., & Silva, C. (2006b). Phonological abilities and writing among Portuguese preschool children. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 21, 163-182. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03173575

Alves Martins, M., & Silva, C. (2009). Two spelling programmes that promote understanding of the alphabetic principle in preschool children. Journal of Writing Research, 1(3), 225-241.

Bus, A. G., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1999). Phonological awareness and early reading: A meta-analysis of experimental training studies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(3), 403-414. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.403

Cardoso-Martins, C., & Batista, A. (2005). O conhecimento do nome das letras e o desenvolvimento da escrita: Evidência de crianças falantes do português. Psicologia, Reflexão e Crítica, 18, 330-336. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722005000300006

Cardoso-Martins, C., Mesquita, T. C., & Ehri, L. (2011). Letter names and phonological awareness help children to learn letter-sound relations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109, 25-38. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.12.006

Clark, R. E. (2009). How much and what type of guidance is optimal for learning from instruction. In S. Tobias & T. Duffy (Eds.), Implicit theory applied to instruction: Success or failure (pp. 158-183). Routledge.

Cunningham, A. E. (1990). Explicit versus implicit instruction in phonemic awareness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 50(3), 429-444. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(90)90079-N

Dean Jr, D., & Kuhn, D. (2006). Direct instruction vs. discovery. Science Education, 91, 384-397. https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.20194

Ferreiro, E. (1988). L’écriture avant la la lettre. In H. Sinclair (Ed.), La prodution des notations chez le jeune enfant (pp. 18-69). Presses Universitaires de France.

Ferreiro, E. (2002). Escritura y oralidad: Unidades, niveles de análisis y consciencia metalinguistica. In E. Ferreiro (Ed.), Relaciones de (in)dependencia entre oralidad y escritura. Geddisa Editorial.

Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J., Shaywitz, S. E., Shaywitz, B. A., & Fletcher, J. M. (1997). The case for early reading intervention. In B. A. Blachman (Ed.), Foundations of reading acquisition and dyslexia: Implications for early intervention. Erlbaum Associates.

Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: An analysis of the failure of implicit, discovery, problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist, 41(2), 75-86. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4102_1

Kuhn, D. (2005). Education for thinking. Harvard University Press.

Kuhn, D. (2007). Is direct instruction an answer to the right question?. Educational Psychologist, 42(2), 109-113. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520701263376

Kuhn, D. (2011). What is scientific thinking and how does it develop? In U. Goswami (Ed.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (2nd ed., pp. 497-523). Wiley-Blackwell.

Levin, I., Shatil-Carmon, S., & Asif-Rave, O. (2006). Learning of letter names and sounds and their contribution to word recognition. Journal of Child Psychology, 93, 139-165. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2005.08.002

Mann, V. A. (1993). Phoneme awareness and future reading ability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26, 259-269. https://doi.org/10.1177/002221949302600404

McBride-Chang, C. (1998). The development of invented spelling. Early Education and Development, 9, 147-160. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15566935eed0902_3

McBride-Chang, C. (1999). The ABCs of the ABCs: The development of letter-name and letter-sound knowledge. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45(2), 285-308.

Nie, Y., & Lau, S. (2010). Differential relations of implicit and didactic instruction to students cognition, motivation, and achievement. Learning and Instruction, 20, 411-423. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2009.04.002

Ouellette, G., & Sénéchal, M. (2008a). A window into early literacy: Exploring the cognitive and linguistic underpinnings of invented spelling. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12(2), 195-219. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888430801917XXX

Ouellette, G., & Sénéchal, M. (2008b). Pathways to literacy: A study of invented spelling and its role in learning to read. Child Development, 79(4), 899-913. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01164.x

Ouellette, G., & Sénéchal, M. (2016). Invented spelling in kindergarten as a predictor of reading and spelling in grade 1: A new pathway to literacy, or just the same road, less known?. Developmental Psychology, 53(1), 77-88. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000179

Ouellette, G., Sénéchal, M., & Haley, A. (2013). Guiding children’s invented spellings: A gateway into literacy learning. The Journal of Experimental Education, 81(2), 261-279. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220973.2012.699903

Pontecorvo, C., & Orsolini, M. (1996). Writing and written language in children’s development. In C. Pontecorvo, M. Orsolini, B. Burge, & L. Resnick (Eds.), Children’s early text construction (pp. 3-23). Lawrence Erlbaum.

Pulido L., & Morin M. F. (2017). Invented spelling: What is the best way to improve literacy skills in kindergarten?. Educational Psychology, 38, 980-996. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2017.1414155

Quinteros, G. (1994). El uso y función de las letras en el período pre-alfabético. Lectura y Vida, 15, 28-38. http://www.lecturayvida.fahce.unlp.edu.ar/numeros/a15n4/15_04_Quinteros.pdf

Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (1998). Manual Raven matrices progressivas (3ª ed.). TEA Ediciones, S.A.

Read, C. (1971). Pre-school children’s knowledge of English phonology. Harvard Educational Review, 41, 1-34. https://doi.org/10.17763/haer.41.1.91367v0h80051573

Richardson, V. (2003). Implicit pedagogy. Teacher College Record, 105(9), 1623-1640. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1467-9620.2003.00303

Richgels, D. (1995). Invented spelling ability and printed word learning in kindergarten. Reading Research Quarterly, 30, 96-109. https://doi.org/10.2307/747746

Rieben, L., Ntamakiliro, L., Gonthier, B., & Fayol, M. (2005). Effects of various early writing practices on reading and spelling. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9(2), 145-166.

Santos, A. I., & Alves Martins, M. (2011). Literacy practices in Portuguese kindergartens and children’s conceptualizations about written language. Revue les Dossiers des Sciences de l’Éducation, 25, 125-141. https://doi.org/10.4000/dse.1043

Shatil, E., Share, D. L., & Levin, I. (2000). On the contribution of kindergarten writing to grade 1 literacy: A longitudinal study in Hebrew. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 1-21. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400001016

Silva, C. (2002). Bateria de provas fonológicas. ISPA – Instituto Universitário.

Silva, C., Almeida, T., & Alves Martins, M. (2010). The letter’s names and the letter’s sound: It’s implication for the phonetization process. Reading and Writing, 23(2), 147-172. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-008-9157-3

Sweller, J. (2004). Instructional design consequences of an analogy between evolution by natural selection and human cognitive architecture. Instructional Science, 32(1), 9-31. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:TRUC.0000021808.72598.4d

Sweller, J. (2012). Human cognitive architecture: Why some instructional procedures work and others do not. In K. Harris, S. Graham, & T. Urdan (Eds.), APA educational psychology handbook: Theories, constructs, and critical issues (Vol. 1, pp. 295-325). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/13273-011

Sweller, J., Kirschner, P. A., & Clark, R. E. (2007). Why minimally guided teaching techniques do not work: A reply to commentaries. Educational Psychology, 42(2), 115-121. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520701263426

Tolchinsky, L. (2005). The emergence of writing. In C. MacArthur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. 83-96). Guilford.

Tolchinsky, L., & Teberosky, A. (1998). The development of word segmentation and writing in two scripts. Cognitive Development, 13(1), 1-24. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(98)90018-1

Treiman, R. (1994). Use of consonant letters names in beginning spelling. Developmental Psychology, 30(4), 567-580. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.30.4.567

Treiman, R. (1998). Why spelling? The benefits of incorporating spelling into beginning to reading instruction. In J. L. Metsala & L. C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 289-313). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Vigário, M., Martins, F., & Frota, S. (2006). A ferramenta FreP e a frequência de tipos silábicos e de classes de segmentos no Português. In F. Oliveira & J. Barbosa (Orgs.), Textos seleccionados do XXI Encontro Nacional da Associação Portuguesa de Linguística (pp. 675-687). APL.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Harvard University Press.




DOI: https://doi.org/10.14417/ap.1848

Refbacks

  • There are currently no refbacks.


Nº ERC: 107494 | ISSN (in print): 0870-8231 | ISSN (online): 1646-6020 | Copyright © ISPA - CRL, 2012 | Rua Jardim do Tabaco, 34, 1149-041 Lisboa | NIF: 501313672 | The portal and metadata are licensed under the license Creative Commons CC BY-NC